http://dx.doi.org/10.20511/pyr2018.v6n2.216
REASEARCH ARTICLE
Evidence of Validity and Factorial Invariance of a Brief Jealousy Scale in Peruvian University Students
Evidencias de validez e invarianza factorial de una Escala Breve de Celos en estudiantes universitarios Peruanos
José Ventura-León1, Tomás Caycho-Rodríguez1, Miguel Barboza-Palomino1, Victor Aparco1, Nikolai Rodas2 |
|
1. Universidad Privada del Norte, Lima, Perú. 2. Universidad César Vallejo, Lima, Perú. |
Summary
The study sought to analyze the evidences of validity, factorial invariance and reliability of the scores of the Jealousy Subscale of the Inventory of Emotional Communication in Romantic Relationships (Sánchez, 2012). To that effect, there was a sample of 1176 Peruvian university students who completed the 11 items of the original version. The confirmatory factor analysis verified a satisfactory adjustment of a final model of nine items in a single factor and correlated errors. It is concluded that the Jealousy scale is a measure that can be used in the professional and research activity, obtaining valid interpretations, reliable and invariant scores according to gender in Peruvian university students.
Keywords: Jealousy, couple relationships, university, instrumental study, validation, factor analysis, invariance.
Resumen
El estudio buscó analizar las evidencias de validez, invarianza factorial y fiabilidad de las puntuaciones de la Subescala de Celos del Inventario de Comunicación Emocional en las Relaciones Románticas (Sánchez, 2012). Para ello se contó con una muestra de 1176 estudiantes universitarios peruanos que completaron los 11 ítems de la versión original. El análisis factorial confirmatorio verificó un ajuste satisfactorio de un modelo final de nueve ítems en un único factor y errores correlacionados. Se concluye que la escala de Celos constituye una medida breve que puede emplearse en la actividad profesional e investigativa, obteniendo interpretaciones válidas, puntuaciones fiables e invariantes de acuerdo con el sexo en estudiantes universitarios peruanos.
Palabras clave: Celos; Relaciones de pareja; Universitarios; Estudio instrumental; Validación; Análisis factorial; Invarianza.
Introduction
There is currently a large number of research works focused on jealousy (Elphinston, Feeney, & Noller, 2011). International figures indicate that in a situation described as "lack of love", jealousy and indifference are experienced by 70% of women and 58% of men (Cantera, Estébanez, & Vázquez, 2009). American marriage and family therapists also report that a third of their clients have jealousy problems (White, 2008), which are often approached under a cognitive (Cuesta, 2006) or cognitive-behavioral model (Martínez-León et al., 2016).
Different studies indicate that jealousy has different implications in the personal sphere and in the society in general (Kelley, Eastwick, Harmon- Jones, & Schmeichel, 2015). Thus, for example, jealousy decreases satisfaction within the couple relationship (Barelds & Barelds-Dijkstra, 2007; Bevan 2008; Parker, Low, Walker, & Gamm 2005) and in relationships with close others (Andersen, Eloy, Guerrero & Spitzberg, 1995). In addition, jealousy is a predictor of aggression for both men and women within a couple relationship, which can even lead to murder (Caldwell, Swan, Allen, Sullivan, & Snow, 2009; Canto, García, & Gómez, 2009; Fenton & Rathus, 2010; Finkel, 2007; Foran & O’Leary, 2008; Gage & Hutchinson, 2006; Guerrero, Hannawa, & Babin, 2011; Kar & O’Leary, 2013; O’Leary, Smith Slep, & O’Leary, 2007; Wright, 2017).
In Peru, a study conducted between 2011 and 2015 (National Institute of Statistics and Informatics [INEI, by its Spanish initials], 2015) indicates that jealousy is the second cause of femicide (39.3%); while police reports reveal that it is the first cause of this crime (46%) (Ministry of the Interior of Peru [MININTER], 2017). This leads to the consideration of jealousy as a risk factor for gender-based violence (Central America Regional Office of the United Nations High Commissioner for Human Rights [OHCHR], 2014). This situation occurs because of a passionate idea of love that involves showing jealousy and despair of the other person (Masters, Johnson and Kolodny, 1987), which added to a hegemonic masculinity makes men have more controlling behavior towards women (Ramírez, 2000; Borraz, 2016). From a psychological perspective, this may be due to the accentuated prohibition against expressing anger in childhood (Singer & Singer, 1993; Hidalgo, 2010) and the erroneous interpretation of reality (Beck, 1990).
An important point for the identification and measurement of jealousy is its adequate conceptualization (Elphinston, et al., 2011). Although different definitions of jealousy can be found, it is generally considered as a human emotion typical of romantic relationships, which is universally experienced by individuals, regardless of their age, sexual orientation, social class, culture and type of relationship (Bernhard, 1986; De Silva, 2004; De Steno, Valdesolo, & Bartlett, 2006). Jealousy arises from the real or imaginary suspicion of threat or loss of affection within a relationship considered valuable (Canto- Ortiz, García-Leiva, & Gómez, 2009; Echeburúa & Fernández-Montalvo,
2001) and is composed of a set of emotions such as pain, anxiety and anger (Parrott & Smith, 1993). People who experience high levels of jealousy have a set of psychological characteristics such as low self-esteem, neuroticism, anxious attachment, and feelings of dependence and sense of possession of the partner (Pines, 1998).
According to some authors (Elphinston, et al., 2011; Guerrero, Spitzberg, & Yoshimura, 2004; Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989), jealousy can have cognitive, emotional, and behavioral expression. The cognitive expression of jealousy includes suspicions, thoughts, and concerns about the probable attraction of the partner towards another person (Echeburua, Amor & Corral, 2009; Monroy, et al., 2015; Pfeiffer & Wong 1989), as well as catastrophic anticipation and unattractive personal thoughts (Cuesta, 2006). For its part, emotional jealousy is a set of anticipated affective responses to threats, such as fear, sadness, anger, envy (Guerrero, Trost, & Yoshimura 2005) and emotional dependence (Cuesta, 2006), which affects the way to communicate and face jealousy (Guerrero & Andersen 1998). Behavioral jealousy includes the evident expression of jealousy (Pfeiffer & Wong 1989), which can be questions (Cuesta, 2006).
A review of the literature (White & Mullen, 1989) presents three types of jealousy as clinical problems. The first one is the normal jealousy that is common and is related to the presence of a real threat to the romantic relationship. The second one is the symptomatic jealousy where the perception of threat to the romantic relationship is associated with pathologies such as schizophrenia, paranoia, and alcohol and drug dependence. Finally, pathological jealousy involves exaggerated responses to a real threat, caused by temporary or chronic self-esteem problems, traumas from experiences of infidelity or personality disorders. Other classifications indicate the existence of delirious paranoid jealousy, sexual jealousy, justified and unjustified jealousy, reactive and suspicious jealousy, among others (Monroy, et al., 2015).
On the other hand, several studies suggest differences according to gender in jealousy between men and women (Bendixen, Kennair & Buss, 2015; Brase, Adair & Monk, 2014; Frederick & Fales, 2014; Zengel, Edlund & Sagarin, 2012). In this sense, in the Scandinavian context the presence of large differences between both genders with respect to the responses of jealousy is reported (Bendixen et al., 2015; Kennair, Nordeide, Andreassen, Strønen, & Pallesen, 2011). From an evolutionary point of view, men and women present differences in sexual and emotional jealousy (DeSteno, Bartlett & Salovey, 2002). Sexual jealousy results from the knowledge of or suspicion that the partner is having an affair with another person, while emotional jealousy is the result of the knowledge or suspicion of the partner’s emotional attachment to another person (Demirtaş-Madran, 2008). Likewise, men experience greater sexual jealousy and women experience a greater amount of emotional jealousy (Edalati & Redzuan, 2010; Fussell, 2012; Varga, Gee, & Munro, 2011). Other research works, on the other hand, report that there are no gender differences in the expression of jealousy (Carpenter 2012; Demirtaş & Dönmez 2006).
Women tend to express jealousy accompanied by sadness or depression, while men by anger or aggression (Alario, 2002). From a social cognitive perspective (Harris 2003), differences in jealousy between men and women result from threats to self-concept, as well as the influence of cultural norms and sexual roles and the beliefs associated with these roles (Salovey & Rothman 1991; Ward & Voracek 2004). In this same sense, some studies prove that the different income levels among the members of the couple, the duration of the relationship and the previous experiences of infidelity in romantic relationships cause marked differences in jealousy (Frederick & Fales, 2014; Murphy, Vallacher, Shackelford, Bjorklund, & Yunger, 2006). On the other hand, marital status (single, married, among others) does not seem to have a moderating effect on gender differences related to jealousy (Zengel et al., 2012), although other studies show that single women are more jealous than married women. Likewise, the latter are more jealous than married men (Demirtaş, 2004). Finally, in relation to age, older women are more emotionally jealous than their husbands, while younger women are more sexually jealous than their partners (Shackelford, et al., 2004).
The measurement of jealousy is an emerging issue, and Pfeiffer and Wong’s three-dimensional jealousy scale (1989) composed of 24 items, validated in Australia (Elphinston, Feeney, & Noller, 2011), Brazil (Lucas, Pereira, Esgalhado, 2012) and Italy (Tani & Ponti, 2016) is used worldwide, while a Multidimensional Inventory of Romantic Jealousy has recently been developed in Latin America, which is composed of 150 items, with five second-order and thirteen first-order scales (Palma, Fuentes, Medina, Escobar, & Vergara, 2016), although its high number of questions and factors makes it complex to replicate. On the other hand, there is the emotional jealousy scale of Kizildag and Yildirim (2017), which presents two methodological errors: (a) it estimates a total alpha (α = .95) and (b) it does not check a bifactor model despite the high inter-factor correlation. Finally, there is the one-dimensional scale of jealousy provocation with 18 items (Cayanus & Booth-Butterfield, 2004). In Peru, there is only one recent study about a six-dimensional scale of 31 items (Mendieta, 2018), a situation that is strange because jealousy is the first cause of femicide in Peru (MININTER, 2017). In this sense, it is necessary to validate the jealousy subscale of the Inventory of Emotional Communication in Romantic Relationships which has an interpersonal approach and measures the emotional experience in relation to the behaviors of the couple that spark the emotion of jealousy in the individual (Sánchez, 2012).
Therefore, the main objective of the study is to analyze the evidence of the validity of internal structure, content, and the invariance of measurement and reliability of a brief measure of Jealousy in Peruvian university students who are in a relationship. Having an instrument validated for the Peruvian context will help to obtain information to better understand jealousy. It will also make possible the analysis of the relationships of jealousy with other variables, the development of adaptations to the characteristics of different samples and the development of cross-cultural research works.
Method Design
It is a study with instrumental design as it aims to review the psychometric properties of a self-reported measurement instrument (Ato, López, & Benavente, 2013).
Participants
This study had the participation of 1176 Peruvian university students, 879 were women and 297 were men, aged 16-54 (Median = 21.29; DE = 3.911). Likewise, an inclusion criterion was that people had a romantic relationship of at least one month in duration. Table 1 describes some socio-demographic characteristics of the participants. Likewise, in order to examine the measurement invariance, 297 women were randomly selected from the total number of women (using a computational method) and they were contrasted with the 297 men in the study. Thus, there were 594 participants.
Instrument
The Inventory of Emotional Communication in Romantic Relationships (Sánchez, 2012) is composed of eight factors that account for 49.3% of the variance of the model. They include: Love-Happiness (α = .97), Passion (α = .93), Jealousy (α = .88), Fear (α = .89), Sadness (α = .88), Anger (α = .86), Positive Surprise (α = .79), Negative Surprise (α = .70) that are supposed to be related [information on the inter-factor correlation is not provided]. Factorial loads were ≥.40. The Jealousy Subscale is composed of 11 items with Likert-type response alternatives from 1 to 5 with expressions: "Never","Sometimes", "Frequently", "Many times", "Always". According to the results of Sánchez (2012), conceptually, the scale measures behaviors that put the couple relationship at risk because of a third party and its reliability using the alpha coefficient can be considered good.
Procedure
Initially, the understanding of the items was verified through a pilot study with 10 people in a couple relationship. Based on this, the items were modified (see Table 2). Then, the test was submitted to the scrutiny of three expert judges to verify its conceptual content, observing items 7 and 10 because they measured infidelity rather than jealousy. In this sense, it was decided to administer the scale with 11 items to observe the statistical behavior of items 7 and 10. Finally, the measurement instrument was applied in two modalities: (a) face-to-face (65%), surveying university students in the surrounding area of their university and in common areas of their university (student breakout areas, green areas, cafeteria, etc.); (b) virtual (35%), using an online form shared through social media.
With respect to ethical issues, an informed consent explaining the conditions of anonymity, voluntary participation and the objective of the study, as well as the use of possible results, was given prior to the completion of the questionnaire.
Data Analysis
The statistical analyses were carried out using the R 3.1.2 software (R Development Core Team, 2007), specifically using the "lavaan" package (Rosseel et al., 2018). The analysis was carried out in stages: In a first stage, the items were analyzed descriptively: arithmetic mean, standard deviation, asymmetry and kurtosis (Hair, Anderson, Tatham & Black, 2005). For these last two coefficients, +/- 1.5 was considered as an indicator of high values (Forero, Maydeu-Olivares, & Gallardo-Pujol, 2009).
In a second stage, the Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was carried out. The multivariate normality assumption was confirmed using the Mardia’s coefficient (1970), which, being higher than 70, can be considered non-normal (Rodríguez & Ruiz, 2008). The weighted least squares with mean, variance adjusted and the Satterthwaite approximation (WLSMVS) estimator was used due to the ordinal nature of the data and differences in variances (Brown, 2006; Rosseel et al., 2018). The goodness-of-fit measures were used to assess the models (Browne & Cudeck, 1993; Hu & Bentler,1999; Mueller and Hancock, 2008): RMSEA (≤ .08), SRMR (≤ .06), CFI (≥.95) and ≥/g1 with values ranging from 2 (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2007) to 5 (Wheaton, Muthen, Alwin, & Summers, 1977).
In a third stage, the factorial invariance of the jealousy scale by gender was tested. In this sense, the invariance was assessed by levels (Byrne, 2008; Vandenberg & Lance, 2000): Configurational (M1), which suggests the factorial structure without restrictions, being considered as the baseline; Metric (M2), where factorial load equivalence restrictions are established; Strong (M3), load and intercept equivalence restrictions, and Strict (M4), load, intercept and residue equivalence restrictions. The measurement invariance and its levels were assessed following the recommendations given by Cheung & Rensvold (2002) ΔCFI ≤ .01 and ΔRMSEA ≤ .015.
Finally, the reliability of the scores was estimated using the Omega coefficient (ω) because a factorial model was used (McDonald, 1999), and their respective confidence intervals were estimated using bootstrapping methods (Ventura-León, 2017).
Results
Preliminary Item Analysis
Table 3 presents the descriptive analysis of the items. Item 5 (M = 4.37; σ = .985) and item 1 (M = 3.33; σ = 1.132) have the highest and the lowest mean values, respectively. Likewise, items 5, 6 and 10 present high asymmetry and kurtosis, being higher +/- 1.5 as an indicator of high values (Forero, Maydeu- Olivares, & Gallardo-Pujol, 2009).
Confirmatory Facto Analysis.
Table 4 presents the CFA used to verify and compare internal structure models of the jealousy scale. The Mardia’s coefficient was 124.369. Thus, the data follow a multivariate non-normal distribution (Rodríguez & Ruiz, 2008). Three factorial structures were modeled: (a) Model 1, with eleven items and a single factor; (b) Model 2, which is a re-specification of model 1 based on the visualization of the error variances of items 6 and 10, a situation that coincides with the observations of the judges who maintained that both items evaluated something other than jealousy; (c) Model 3, without items 6 and 10 and with three correlated measurement errors that were reported by the modification indexes, but following the consideration of theoretical rationality (Boomsma, 2000). In this sense, model 3 is a one-dimensional factorial structure with nine items and three correlated errors. The results indicate that this model has goodness of fit values considered good (García, 2011): (χ (18) = 172.188; p > .05; CFI = .973; SRMR = .028; RMSEA = .085, IC [.070, 102]).
Factorial Invariance
Table 5 shows the invariance measures by gender. Initially, the general population exhibited good goodness of fit values (n=594) (García, 2011). This is also the case when dividing into subgroups (women and men). But, women have higher SRMR and CFI values as compared to those of men. Next, the configurational invariance (M1) was analyzed and exhibited good goodness of fit values χ2= 60.058, CFI =.972; SRMR =.037 and RMSEA = .097 (.073, .122). The M1 is the baseline used to contain the other models with M2, M3, M4 restrictions. Then, the metric invariance (M2), being understood as an M1 with factorial load restrictions, was analyzed and exhibited good goodness of fit indexes: CFI = .970 and RMSEA = .087 (.066, .109), which are similar to the M1 values as they show minimum differences ΔCFI ≤ .01 and ΔRMSEA ≤ .015. Thus, it can be concluded that the factorial loads are equivalent, and therefore, the co-variances can be compared. Subsequently, the threshold equivalence was assessed (strong invariance, M3), showing that the indexes are similar to the previous model: CFI = .978, SRMR = .033 and RMSEA = .059 (.040, .077). The differences with the M2 model are minimal (ΔCFI ≤ .01 and ΔRMSEA ≤ .015; Cheung & Rensvold, 2002), confirming the threshold invariance. In the next step, the strict invariance (M4) was examined and the residue invariance is added. The results indicate that the differences with the M3 model are minimal, confirming the residual invariance.
Once the measurement invariance was verified, the latent means were calculated due to evidence of presence of strong invariance (Dimitrov, 2010). Thus, the mean of the first group was set equal to zero and that of the second group was set free. The results reveal that the latent means of women (4,016) and men (3,922) are not statistically significant and their effect size is non- existent (d = 093; Cohen, 1992).
Reliability
Table 6 presents the factorial loads and reliability estimation using the omega coefficient for both models. However, model 2 exhibits a corrected omega for correlated errors (Raykov, 2001; Komaroff, 1997). In this sense, it can be considered that the internal consistency between the items is good (Cicchetti, 1994).
Discussion
This instrumental-type study joins the research works that are addressing the issue of jealousy in couple relationships (Elphiston et al., 2011). Specifically, the evidence of validity based on internal structure and content was analyzed. In addition, the factorial invariance by gender was examined and the reliability of the scores of the Jealousy Subscale of the Inventory of Emotional Communication in Romantic Relationships was estimated (Sánchez, 2012).
The CFA reveals better goodness of fit values of Model 3 (Re-specified Model), a one-dimensional nine-item model, where items 6 and 10 were removed. Also, it exhibited correlated errors (e1-e2; e7-e8; e8-e9). It should be noted that items 6 and 10 caused distortion in the data because they are not coherent because (according to the expert opinion) they were measuring something other than jealousy. At the same time, from an empirical point of view (factor analysis), they exhibited an excessive error variance due to wording regarding infidelity. In this sense, there was sufficient evidence to remove them from the subscale. On the other hand, the modification index suggested the presence of redundant items. In this sense, some error variances were correlated, resulting in free parameters ranging from .323 to .552.
This version of the Jealousy Subscale is easy to implement in the scientific field, and can be used, for example, in research on couple relationships. This can help to systematize information in the national context that can be discussed with what has been reported in other scenarios (e.g. White, 2008). It also opens the possibility of continuing to adapt the instrument to different samples, taking into account the cultural diversity of Peru and including, in addition to validity and reliability, equity as the measurement property of the functioning of the instrument (Ventura-León, Barboza-Palomino, & Caycho, 2017). Similarly, the instrument can be adapted in different countries of the region, making it possible to develop cross-cultural studies in the future.
Taking into account that jealousy is universally experienced and is present in romantic relationships (Bernhard, 1986; De Silva, 2004; De Steno et al., 2006), it is necessary to investigate how the variable is shown according to different socio-demographic and cultural characteristics. In this sense, as the scale measurement invariance has been exhibited, evidence on jealousy by gender can be generated, as done in previous studies (Bendixen et al., 2015; Brase et al., 2014; Carpenter, 2012; Demirtas & Dönmez, 2006; Edalati & Redzuan, 2010; Frederick & Fales, 2014; Fusell, 2012; Kennair et al., 2011; Varga et al., 2011; Zengel et al., 2012).
The validation of the jealousy scale will also make it possible to examine its relation with other variables. Thus, it is important to know the association with other constructs such as satisfaction with the couple relationship (Barelds & Barelds-Dijkstra, 2007; Bevan, 2008; Parker et al., 2005), depression, sadness, anger (Alario, 2002), infidelity (Murphy et al., 2006), self-concept and beliefs associated with the assigned roles (Ward & Voracek, 2004), among others in the Peruvian context.
Similarly, predictive studies can be developed to value the influence of jealousy on aggression exerted in couple relationships. This will make it possible to corroborate what has been reported in previous studies (Caldwell, Swan, Allen, Sullivan, & Snow, 2009; Canto, García, & Gómez, 2009; Fenton & Rathus, 2010; Finkel, 2007; Foran & O’Leary, 2008; Gage & Hutchinson, 2006; Guerrero, Hannawa, & Babin, 2011; Kar & O’Leary, 2013; O’Leary, Smith Slep, & O’Leary, 2007; Wright, 2017).
This is important because different institutions reveal (INEI, 2015; MININTER, 2017) that jealousy has emerged as a cause of gender violence and femicide. Having an adapted and valid instrument will make it possible to estimate statistical data with more accuracy, and to reveal the social impact of jealousy and systematize it through scientific studies on this variable that remain limited in the Peruvian context.
Although the findings in this study are interesting, there are limitations such as the selection of the sample that was not probabilistic, and that the analysis was limited to two sources of validity: based on the content and the internal structure. It is suggested to explore the functioning with other variables and to examine the reliability using other methods such as test- retest in order to observe the stability of the test over time.
In conclusion, the results make it possible to point out that the Jealousy scale is a brief measure that offers valid interpretations and reliable scores with respect to jealousy in Peruvian university students, in addition to showing invariant according to gender. Therefore, the jealousy scale is a useful tool for the professional practice and the development of new research works.
References
Alario, S. (2002). Tratamiento conductual de un trastorno de celos patológicos. Cuadernos de Medicina Psicosomática y Psiquiatria de Enlace, 62-63, 83-95.
Andersen, P. A., Eloy, S. V., Guerrero, L. K., & Spitzberg, B. H. (1995). Romantic jealousy and relational satisfaction: A look at the impact jealousy experience and expression. Communication Reports, 8(2), 7785. Doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/08934219509367613
Ato, M., López, J. J., & Benavente, A. (2013). Un sistema de clasificación de los diseños de investigación en psicología. Anales de psicología, 29(3), 1038-1059. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.29.3.178511
Barelds, D. P. H., & Barelds-Dijkstra, P. (2007). Relations between different types of jealousy and self and partner perceptions of relationship quality. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 14(3), 176188. Doi: https://doi.org/10.1002=cpp.532
Beck, A. (1990). Con el amor no basta. Barcelona: Paidós.
Bendixen, M., Kennair, L. E., & Buss, D. M. (2015). Jealousy: Evidence of strong sex differences using both forced choice and continuous measure paradigms. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 212-216. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.05.035
Bentler, P. M. (2006). EQS structural equations program manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software.
Bernhard, K. F. (1986). Psychology roots. In K. F. Benard (Ed.), Jealousy: Its nature and treatment (pp. 3552). Springfield, IL: Thomas
Bevan J., L. (2008) Experiencing and communicating romantic jealousy: Questioning the investment model. Southern Communication Journal, 73(1), 4267. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10417940701815626
Borraz, M. (2016, 8 de febrero). El hombre protector, los celos como amor y otros mitos que perpetúan el machismo entre los jóvenes. El Diario. Recuperado de https://www.eldiario.es/sociedad/estereotipos-alimentan- violencia-machista-adolescentes_0_482252121.html
Brase, G., Adair, L., & Monk, K. (2014). Explaining sex differences in reactions to relationship infidelities: Comparisons of the roles of s ex, g ender, beliefs, attachment, and sociosexual orientation. Evolutionary Psychology, 12(1), 73-96. Doi: http://dx.doi. org/10.1177/147470491401200106
Caldwell, J.E., Swan, S.C., Allen, C.T., Sullivan, T.P., & D.L. Snow. (2009).
Why I hit him: Women’s reasons for intimate partner violence. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 18(7), 672-697. Doi: http:// dx.doi.org/10.1080/10926770903231783
Cantera, I., Estébanez, I. & Vázquez, N. (2009). Violencia contra las mujeres jóvenes: la violencia psicológica en las relaciones de noviazgo. Informe Final. Bilbao: Servicio de mujer Módulo Deusto-San Ignacio.
Canto-Ortiz, J. M. C., García-Leiva, P. G., & Jacinto, L. (2009). Celos y emociones: Factores de la relación de pareja en la reacción ante la infidelidad. Athenea Digital, 55(primavera), 3955.
Carpenter, C. J. (2012). Meta-analyses of sex differences in responses to sexual versus emotional infidelity: men and women are more similar than different. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 36(1), 2537. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0361684311414537
Cayanus, J. L., & Booth‐Butterfield, M. (2004). Relationship orientation, jealousy, and equity: An examination of jealousy evoking and positive communicative responses. Communication Quarterly, 52(3), 237-250.
Cicchetti, D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychological assessment, 6(4), 284-290.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155 159. Cuesta, T. (2006). Intervención cognitiva en un caso de celotipia. Acción Psicológica, 4(1), 71-82.
Demirtaş, A. (2004). Yakın ilişkilerde kıskançlık (bireysel, ilişkisel ve durumsal değişkenler) [Jealousy (individual, relational, and situational) in close relationships] (Doctoral dissertation, Ankara University, Ankara, Turkey). Retrieved from https://tez.yok.gov.tr/UlusalTezMerkezi/
Demirtaş, H. A., & Dönmez, A. (2006). Yakın ilişkilerde kıskançlık: Bireysel, ilişkisel ve durumsal değişkenler [Jealousy in close relationships: Personal, relational, and situational variables]. Türk Psikiyatri Dergisi, 17(3), 181191
Demirtaş-Madran, A. (2008). Duygusal ve cinsel kıskançlık açısından temel cinsiyet farklılıkları: Evrimsel yaklaşım ve süregelen tartışmalar [Basic gender differences in terms of emotional and sexual jealousy: The evolutionary approach and ongoing debates]. Türk Psikiyatri Dergisi, 19(3), 300309.
DeSteno, D., Bartlett, M. Y., & Salovey, P. (2002). Sex differences in jealousy: Evolutionary mechanism or artifact of measurement? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(5), 11031116. Doi: http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.83.5.1103
DeSteno, D., Valdesolo, P., & Barlett, M. (2006). Jealousy and the threatened self: getting to the heart of the green-eyed monster. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 626-641. Doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.626
Dimitrov, D. M. (2010). Testing for factorial invariance in the context of construct validation. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 43(2), 121149. Doi: http://dx.doi. org/10.1177/0748175610373459
Echeburúa, E., & Fernández-Montalvo, J. (2001). Celos en la pareja: una emoción destructiva. España: Ariel.
Echeburúa, E., Amor, P., & De Corral, P. (2009). Hombres violentos contra la pareja: trastornos mentales y perfiles tipológicos. Pensamiento Psicológico, 6(13), 27-36
Edalati, A., & Redzuan, M. (2010). The relationship between jealousy and agression: A review of literatures related to wives’ aggression. European Journal of Scientific Research, 39(4), 498504.
Elphinston, R. A., Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (2011). Measuring romantic jealousy: Validation of the multidimensional jealousy scale in Australian samples. Australian Journal of Psychology, 63(4), 243251. Doi: http:// dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1742-9536.2011.00026.x
Elphinston, R. A., Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (2011). Measuring romantic jealousy: Validation of the multidimensional jealousy scale in Australian samples. Australian Journal of Psychology, 63(4), 243-251.
Fenton, B. & J.H. Rathus. (2010). Men’s self-reported descriptions and precipitants of domestic violence perpetration as reported in intake evaluations. Journal of Family Violence, 25(2), 149-158. Doi: http:// dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10896-009-9278-8
Finkel, E. J. (2007). Impelling and inhibiting forces in the perpetration of intimate partner violence. Review of General Psychology, 11(2), 193. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.193
Foran, H.M. & K.D. O’Leary. (2008). Problem drinking, jealousy, and anger control: Variables predicting physical aggression against a partner. Journal of Family Violence, 23(3), 141-148. Doi: http://dx.doi. org/10.1007/s10896-007-9136-5
Forero, C. G., Maydeu-Olivares, A., & Gallardo-Pujol, D. (2009). Factor analysis with ordinal indicators: A Monte Carlo study comparing DWLS and ULS estimation. Structural Equation Modeling, 16(4), 625-641.
Frederick, D. A., & Fales, M. R. (2014). Upset over sexual versus emotional infidelity among gay, lesbian, bisexual, and heterosexual adults. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45(1), 175-191. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0409-9
Fussell, N. J. (2012). Between-sex differences in romantic jealousy: Substance or spin? A qualitative analysis. Evolutionary Psychology, 10(1), 136172. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/147470491201000114
Gage, A., & Hutchinson, P. (2006). Power, control and intimate partner sexual violence in Haiti. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35(1), 11-24. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-006-8991-0
García, M. (2011). Análisis causal con ecuaciones estructurales de la satisfacción ciudadana con los servicios municipales (Tesis de Maestría). Universidad Santiago de Compostela, España.
Guerrero, L. K., & Andersen, P. A. (1998). Jealousy experience and expression in romantic relationships. In P. A. Andersen & L. K. Guerrero (Eds.). Handbook of communication and emotion: Research, theory, applications, and contexts (pp. 155188). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Guerrero, L. K., Trost, M.R., & Yoshimura, S. M. (2005). Romantic jealousy: Emotions and communicative responses. Personal Relationships, 12(2), 233-252. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j. 1350-4126.2005.00113.x
Guerrero, L., Spitzberg, B., & Yoshimura, S. (2004). Sexual and emotional jealousy. In J. H. Harvey, A. Wenzel, & S. Sprecher (Eds.), The handbook of sexuality in close relationships (pp. 311345). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Guerrero, L.K., Hannawa, A.F., & E.A. Babin. (2011). The communicative responses to jealousy scale: revision, empirical validation, and associations with relational satisfaction. Communication Methods and Measures, 5(3), 223-249. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19312458.2011.596993
Harris, C. R. (2003). A review of sex differences in sexual jealousy, including self-report data, psychophysiological responses, interpersonal violence, and morbid jealousy. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7(2), 102128. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0702_102-128
Hidalgo, E. (2010). Los celos infantiles. Revista Digital Enfoques Educativos, 55, 168-177.
Instituto Nacional de Estadística e Informática (2015). Perú, Encuesta demográfica y de salud familiar-ENDES 2014. Recuperado de https://www.inei.gob.pe/media/MenuRecursivo/publicaciones_digitales/Est/ Lib1211/pdf/Libro.pdf
Kar H. L., O’Leary K. D. (2013). Patterns of psychological aggression, dominance, and jealousy within marriage. Journal of Family Violence, 28(2), 109-119. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10896-012-9492-7
Kelley N. J., Eastwick P. W., Harmon-Jones E., & Schmeichel B. J. (2015). Jealousy increased by induced relative left frontal cortical activity. Emotion 15(5), 550555. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000068
Kennair, L.E.O., Nordeide, J., Andreassen, S., Strønen, J., & Pallesen, S. (2011). Sex differences in jealousy: A study from Norway. Nordic Psychology, 63, 20-34. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1901-2276/a000025
Kizildag, S., & Yildirim, I. (2017). Developing the Spouse Emotional Jealousy Scale. Educational Sciences: Theory and Practice, 17(1), 175-190.
Lucas, C., Pereira, H., & Esgalhado, G. (2012). Evaluation of Romantic Jealousy: Psychometric Study of the Multidimensional Jealousy Scale for the Portuguese Population. Psychology, Community & Health, 1(2), 151-162.
Martínez-León, N., García-Rincón, L., Valencia, D., Barreto, S., Alfonso, A., Quintero, C. Parra, A. (2016). Efecto de una intervención cognoscitivo conductual para el manejo de los celos en la relación de pareja. Psychologia: Avances de la Disciplina, 10(1), 113-123.
Masters, W., Johnson, V., & Kolodny, R. (1987). La sexualidad humana. Volumen 2. Barcelona: Grijalbo.
Mendieta, R. E. (2018). Evidencias de validez de la Escala multidimensional de celos en estudiantes de superior técnico de la Provincia de Virú (Tesis de pregrado). Universidad Cesar Vallejo, Lima-Perú.
Ministerio del Interior (2017). Boletín estadístico policial primer trimestre 2017. Recuperado de https://www.pnp.gob.pe/anuario_estadistico/documentos/Boletin%202017%20primer%20trimestre%20V2.0.pdf
Monroy, E.J., Sosa, V.S., Castillo, E., Mendoza, A.L., Rubio, E.P. & Amador, R. (2015). Reactivos o sospechosos: construcción y validación de una escala para medir el tipo de celos. Revista Electrónica de Psicología Iztacala, 18(1), 1-12.
Murphy, S. M., Vallacher, R. R., Shackelford, T. K., Bjorklund, D. F., & Yunger, J. L. (2006). Relationship experiences as a predictor of romantic jealousy. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(4), 761-769. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.09.004
O’Leary, K. D., Smith Slep, A. M., & O’Leary, S. G. (2007). Multivariate models of men’s and women’s partner aggression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(5), 752764. Doi: http://dx.doi. org/10.1037/0022-006X.75.5.752
Oficina Regional para América Central del Alto Comisionado de las Naciones Unidas para los Derechos Humanos (2014). Modelo de protocolo latinoamericano de investigación de las muertes violentas de mujeres por razones de género (femicidio/feminicidio). Recuperado de http://www. oacnudh.org/wp-content/uploads/2014/08/Modelo_de_Protocolo.pdf
Palma, J. M., Fuentes, N. I. G. A. L., Medina, J. L. V., Escobar, S. G., & Vergara, G. H. (2016). Construcción y análisis estadístico del Inventario Multidimensional de Celos Románticos: Estudio preliminar. Actualidades en Psicología, 30(120), 31-45.
Parker, J. G., Low, C. M., Walker, A. R., & Gamm, B. K. (2005). Friendship jealousy in young adolescents: Individual differences and links to sex, self-esteem, aggression, and social adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 41(1), 235250. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.235
Parrott, W. G., & Smith, R. H. (1993). Distinguishing the experiences of envy and jealousy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(6), 906-920. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.6.906
Pfeiffer, S. M., & Wong, P. T. (1989). Multidimensional jealousy. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6(2), 181-196.
Pfeiffer, S. M., & Wong, P. T. P. (1989). Multidimensional jealousy. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6(2), 181196. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177=026540758900600203
Pines, A. M. (1998). Romantic jealousy: Causes, symptoms, cures. London, England: Routledge.
Ramírez, F. (2000). Violencia masculina en el hogar. México, D.F.: Pax México.
Rodríguez, M, & Ruiz, M. (2008). Atenuación de la asimetría y de la curtosis de las puntuaciones observadas mediante transformaciones de variables: incidencia sobre la estructura factorial. Psicológica, 29(2), 205-227.
Rosseel, Y., Oberski, D., Byrnes, J., Vanbrabant, L., Savalei, V., Merkle, E., ... & Chow, M. (2017). Package ‘lavaan’. Recuperado el 03 de enero 2018 de https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/e465/ add515cdeeaa8de57bd1a9adc2569e44fa9a.pdf
Rosseel, Y., Oberski, D., Byrnes, J., Vanbrabant, L., Savalei, V., Merkle, E., & Chow, M. (2018). Package ‘lavaan’ 0.6-2. Recuperado de https:// cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lavaan/lavaan.pdf
Salovey, P., & Rothman, A. J. (1991). Envy and jealousy: Self and society. In P. Salovey (Ed.), The psvchology of jealousy and envy (pp. 271-286). New York: Guitford Press
Sánchez, R. (2012). Comunicación Emocional en las Relaciones Románticas: Diseño y Validación de una Medida en México. Interpersona. An International Journal on Personal Relationships 6(2), 211226, Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.5964/ijpr.v6i2.102
Shackelford, T. K., Voracek, M., Schmitt, D. P., Buss, D. M., Weekes- Shackelford, V. A., & Michalski, R. L. (2004). Romantic jealousy in early adulthood and in later life. Human Nature, 15(3), 283300. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12110-004-1010-z
Silva de, P. (2004). Jealousy in couple relationships. Behavior Change, 21(1), 1-13.
Singer, S. & Singer, A. (1993). La Gestalt: una terapia de contacto. México: El Manual Moderno.
Tani, F., & Ponti, L. (2016). The romantic jealousy as multidimensional construct: A study on the Italian Short Form of the Multidimensional Jealousy Scale. The Open Psychology Journal, 9(1).
Vandenberg, R. J., & Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational research methods, 3(1), 4-70.
Varga, C. M., Gee, C. B., & Munro, G. (2011). The effects of sample characteristics and experience with infidelity on romantic jealousy. Sex Roles, 65(11-12), 854866. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-0048-8
Ventura-León, J. L. (2017). Intervalos de confianza para coeficiente Omega: Propuesta para el cálculo [carta al Editor]. Adicciones. Avance de publicación on-line. Doi: https://doi.org/10.20882/adicciones.962
Ventura-León, J. L., Barboza-Palomino, M., & Caycho, T. (2017). ¿Son necesarios los instrumentos equitativos? Educación Médica. Publicación anticipada en línea. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.edumed.2017.03.022
Ward, J., & Voracek, M. (2004). Evolutionary and social cognitive explanations of sex in romantic jealousy. Australian Journal of Psychology, 56(3), 165171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00049530412331283381
Wheaton, B., Muthen, B., Alwin, D., F., & Summers, G. (1977). Assessing Reliability and Stability in Panel Models, Sociological Methodology, 8(1), 84-136. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/270754
White, G.L. (2008). Romantic jealousy: Therapists’ perceptions of causes, consequences,andtreatments.JournalofCoupleandRelationshipTherapy, 7(3), 210-229. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15332690802238019
White, G.L., & Mullen, P. E. (1989). Jealousy: Theory, Research, and Clinical Strategies. New York: Guilford Press.
Wright, M. (2017). Intimate partner aggression and adult attachment insecurity: The mediation of jealousy and anger. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 11(2), 187198. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000097
Zengel, B., Edlund, J.E., & Sagarin, B.J.(2012).Sex differences in jealousy in response to infidelity: Evaluation of demographic moderators in a national random sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(1), 4751. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.08.001